×

Researchers develop 'linguistic thermometer' to sketch human innovation

Physics-based tool maps the pace of language development

45500-year-old cave painting | AFP

Researchers have helped develop a powerful physics-based tool to map the pace of language development and human innovation over thousands of years—even stretching into pre-history before records were kept.

Multi-disciplinary researchers from The University of Manchester, have come together as part of an international team to share their diverse expertise to develop the new model.

The study entitled 'Geospatial distributions reflect temperatures of linguistic feature' authored by Henri Kauhanen, Deepthi Gopal, Tobias Galla and Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero, has been published in the journal Science Advances.

Prof Galla has applied statistical physics—usually used to map atoms or nanoparticles—to help build a mathematically-based model that responds to the evolutionary dynamics of language. Essentially, the forces that drive language change can operate across thousands of years and leave a measurable "geospatial signature", determining how languages of different types are distributed over the surface of the Earth.

"In our model, each language has a collection of properties or features and some of those features are what we describe as 'hot' or 'cold', ” explained Dr Bermúdez-Otero.

"So, if a language puts the object before the verb, then it is relatively likely to get stuck with that order for a long period - so that's a 'cold' feature. In contrast, markers like the English article 'the' come and go a lot faster: they may be here in one historical period, and be gone in the next. In that sense, definite articles are 'hot' features.

"The striking thing is that languages with 'cold' properties tend to form big clumps, whereas languages with 'hot' properties tend to be more scattered geographically."

This method, therefore, works like a thermometer, enabling researchers to retrospectively tell whether one linguistic property is more prone to change in historical time than another. This modelling could also provide a similar benchmark for the pace of change in other social behaviours or practices over time and space.

"For example, suppose that you have a map showing the spatial distribution of some variable cultural practice for which you don't have any historical records—this could be anything, like different rules on marriage or the inheritance of possessions," added Dr Bermúdez-Otero.

"Our method could, in principle, be used to ascertain whether one practice changes in the course of historical time faster than another, ie whether people are more innovative in one area than in another, just by looking at how the present-day variation is distributed in space."

The source data for the linguistic modelling comes from present-day languages and the team relied on The World Atlas of Language Structures (WALS). This records information of 2,676 contemporary languages.

"We were interested in emergent phenomena, such as how large-scale effects, for example, patterns in the distribution of language features arise from relatively simple interactions. This is a common theme in complex systems research,” explained Prof Galla.